How do system dynamic models (SD) capture path dependent and complex evolutionary behaviour in social science analysis?
نویسنده
چکیده
The aim of social scientists is to capture the causal mechanisms that explain behavior of people and groups of people, such as communities, societies or firms. Such an endeavor becomes increasingly difficult as theorizing concerns patterns of behavior. A theory of behavior explains how the cause-effect structure of interaction among specific variables leads to emergent paths of behavior of these variables. Thus, building theories of behavior implies creating a narrative that connects a deep theoretical structure to a repertoire of plausible behaviors that encompass the observed critical events and behaviors. A problem challenging discursive theories of behavior is the quality and robustness of inferred connections between causal structure and emerging behaviors. Equally difficult is to understand how modifications of theoretical assumptions, crystallized into a model, lead to modifications of the phenomenon under study. To make the described endeavor even more challenging, observed patterns of behavior are often produced by path-dependent processes that amplify non-systematic and stochastic disturbances. In this essay, we suggest that the interaction between field research, computer simulation and System Dynamics allows to elicit causal models from the rich texture of everyday life. INTRODUCTION As Legorsol suggests in her study on privatization of land in small-scale African communities, since an observed event or phenomenon in most anthropological studies, as it was privatization in her study, is a “...natural experiment and not a controlled The author acknowledges financial support received within the EU research programme MEDEA ‘Models and their Effects on Development paths: an Ethnographic and comparative Approach to knowledge transmission and livelihood strategies’, Project No. 225670. laboratory one, the causal relation of privatization to behavior should be considered indicative, not conclusive.” (Legorsol, 2005). Accurate information that emerges from an in-depth field study are very powerful indications to elicit a candidate causal structure to explain observed behaviors. Informants provide a vital vehicle to build a repertoire of candidates explanations for an observed event. Yet, collected explanations are interpretations that are necessarily more or less intentionally biased. To select among candidates explanations, the quality and robustness of inferred connection between causal structures and emerging behaviors stands as a decisive factor. As for quality of inference we refer to the case when an inferred causal structure is sufficient to generate an observed pattern of behavior. Robustness implies that researchers are able to distinguish between phenomena that are consequences of specific simultaneous combinations of contextual factors and causal structures that might be, at least partially, transferable as a candidate explanation in other research sites. Human reasoning may face limits in the articulation of the chain of causes and effects that is sufficient to give rise to a specific observed behavior. Thus, discursively presented theories of behavior may overlook variables or causal links that are necessary for a behavior to follow from a causal structure. The issue becomes increasingly crucial when phenomena under study display complex behavioral paths such as oscillations, peaks or bifurcations. Equally difficult is to understand how small modifications of a causal structure, or different calibrations of parameters, lead to modifications of the phenomenon under study. For example, an observed phenomenon may be the unique result of a very specific, hardly reproducible, calibration of a set of contextual circumstances. On the other hand, phenomena observed in different contexts and conveying different behavioral paths may nevertheless be generated by the same underpinning causal structure. This is frequent when observed patterns of behavior are produced by path-dependent processes that amplify non-systematic and stochastic disturbances. In this paper, we suggest that computer-aided simulation experiments may support field researchers in investigating the relative roles of history, contextual circumstances and deep causal structures in generating observed paths of behavior. In particular, we focus our attention on a particular approach to modeling and simulation, System Dynamics. This paper is articulated as follows. First, we expose the gist of our argument: how computer simulation supports theorizing. Then, we briefly describe System Dynamics and explain what peculiarities makes the approach suitable for theorizing in social science. In section three, we show how, when looking closely at behaviors captured in field studies, complexity emerges that calls for an approach informed by system thinking. Finally, we report an example of the use of computer modeling and simulation associated to a field study. Markus and Robey (1988), and Mohr (1982) suggest that when processes of change are under scrutiny, robust theorizing needs adopting an idiographic approach that emphasize how specific contextual assumptions explain unfolding behavior. BUILDING THEORIES OF BEHAVIOR AND CHANGE Building theories of behavior and change implies providing an explanation that infers a relationship between a structure of causes and a number of events, which are connected into an historical sequence. We propose that computer simulation allows to move between natural and virtual experiments to understand how a causal structure is able to explain observed patterns of behavior. The key theme here is the ability of a researcher to enact and maintain a dialogue between theoretical behaviors, as predicted by a simulation, which has been built upon accounts from field studies, and observed empirical patterns. In this light, the use of computer simulation brings about a number of advantages. First, in general, computer simulation may generate inputs in the form of time-series. This may result of some help when time-series can be compared directly with realworld quantitative figures, for example demographic data. In this case, the availability of real and simulated time series that are accessible in a similar quantitative format facilitates pattern-matching thereby allowing researchers to visually assess resemblance between simulated series, which follows from the quantitative simulation of a theoretical hypothesis, and an empirically observed behavior. In this respect, it is possible to generate measures of how simulated events match empirical instances of those events (Sterman 1984). Second, computer simulation allows for a rigorous longitudinal articulation of predicted behaviors. In other words, the computer-aided process of deduction goes far beyond the human capability to appreciate the long-term features of the behavior of selected variables. Thus, computer simulation can support researcher to find plausible sufficient conditions for complex patterns of behavior to happen such as peaks and lowest point, oscillations with different characteristics and changes in rates of growth or decline. Third, researchers, by simulating a formal model, can articulate their predictions by contemporaneously producing behavior of different variables and the interactions of these latter. In particular, researchers can simulate the interaction of independent and dependent variables in each time step, along a given time horizon. This cross-sectional articulation of patterns of behavior increases the points of contacts between a set of behavioral hypotheses and the empirical context of a case study. As Kaplan suggests ‘What counts in the validation of a theory, so far as fitting the facts are concerned, is the convergence of the data brought to bear upon it [...]’ (1964: 314). Thus, a computer simulation expands the terrain where comparison between theory and empirical setting takes place by generating a rich longitudinal and cross-sectional articulation of behavior under study. In this light, the convergence of data and the concatenation of events that is necessary to obtain to use a case study to confirm a theory is increasing demanding. In this respect, computer simulation aids researchers to design field studies to produce difficult experiments where the falsifiability of a theory is easier because fitting the facts becomes increasingly hard. Of course, on the other hand, had empirically collected facts to fit, at least qualitatively, into a complex web of interweaved simulated behaviors, the experiment would lead to stronger evidence to confirm propositions contained in the theory. Following, we suggest two avenues to conduct simulation experiments and articulate a theoretical hypothesis. Sensitivity analysis and history-convergent runs A first possible scenario entails that the hypothesis produced in a field study, once formalized in a computer model, produce behaviors that are similar to those observed in the field. In this case, computer simulation can be used as a laboratory to produce sensitivity analysis. Field cases are retrospective studies. Retrospective studies explain, ex-post how a set of variables interacted to drive an observed behavior of interest. However, it could become troublesome to ascertain the extent to which a theoretical explanatory model and the observed behavior are linked. If, for example, we are aware that two variables affect the observed behavior, given the complex web of interactions in which these variables are embedded, it might be hard to determine their relative strengths. It might be the case that the influence of one of these two variables is insignificant, and could be omitted from the analysis to satisfy the criterion of parsimony for a good theory (Eisenhardt 1990). To further investigate the importance of that variable, an experiment could be run to detect what happens if the variable is omitted from the model. This sensitivity analysis may help revising an explanation by suggesting that specific variables may not be necessary to explain a behavior whereas others are fundamental since the change in their calibrations produces simulated behavior to diverge from the one observed. In addition, the intentional generation of history-divergent simulation runs orients further empirical enquiry by indicating new potential research sites. Indeed, in a new site that resembles the simulation settings that have been adopted in the sensitivity analysis, a researcher can test whether, given the characteristics of the new site, a behavior closer to the history-divergent run is observed. For example, some longitudinal event studies have compared polar cases, that is, objects of study that have shown opposite behaviors in responding to an identical exogenous stimulus, and have explained the different unfolding of their histories as the result of different initial conditions (Noda 1994; Noda and Bower 1996). What we suggest is that using sensitivity analysis to generate history-divergent runs may be helpful to illuminate the potential of a research site to become a polar case in which, given a change in some key features of the research context, a behavior divergent from the one observed in the original field study ensues. In general, simulation, by connecting a theoretical structure to a variety of possible emerging, often unexpected, behaviors, activates dormant consequences of a theory, which were not observed in the original empirical study. This generation of a distribution of near-histories, or unrealized events, both strengthens the understanding of causal structures and envisions areas for further empirical investigations. Field researches conducted in new sites are theoretical experiments that reinforce internal validity of a theory. Thus, computer simulation helps researchers in thinking how a common theoretical engine may explain a repertoire of different behaviors in different empirical contexts. In this vein, the coupling of field-study and computer simulation speaks to the problem of learning from samples of one or fewer as presented by March, Sproull and Tamuz (1991). In this light, the issue of transferability of insight from one research site to another proceeds through a process of déjà vu; observing a pattern that is included within the repertoire of behaviors that are generated by a single causal structure a researcher may receive an hint on candidate explanation for that specific pattern. If we consider a case study as an experiment, computer simulation allows learning from this experiment by exploring how small changes in some conditions generate different behaviors. These behaviors are near-histories that may materialize and become visible in other empirical contexts. Sensitivity analysis and history-divergent runs When comparing computer-simulated and empirical patterns of behavior, a researcher may observe a mismatch. In this case, the problem is to understand why behaviors diverge. In this respect, computer models provide a theoretical laboratory that is relatively easy to manipulate in order to investigate possible explanations of the discrepancy between simulated predictions and observed behaviors. In this respect, we agree with Malerba et al. (1999) in suggesting that computer simulation provide an appropriate terrain to nurture a friendly dialogue between empirical evidence and theory. When history-divergent simulations appear, researcher tries to explain where discrepancies come from. Investigators can intervene on the structure of a computer model or on the calibration of model’s parameters and rigorously deduct whether these interventions narrow down the gap between predicted and actual behaviors. Pressures for historical and simulated behaviors to diverge arise in two cases. The first pressure intervenes when the causal structure of the theory, which is captured in the computer model, is isomorphic to the causal relationships at work in a specific empirical setting and the discrepancy is the consequence of flaws in the specifications of parameters’ calibrations. The second pressure for historical and simulated behaviors to diverge arises when the causal structure of the theory and the causal relationships at work in the real world are not isomorphic in some respects. This may be the result either of the fact that a researcher has not properly formalized a theoretical argument arising from a field study or the fact that the researcher was not able to select the key causal mechanisms at work in the case studied. The first direction to explore is the analysis of sensitivity of model’s behaviors to change in parameters to check whether simulating the model with a new calibration improves the match between simulated and observed behaviors. The fact that the fit between simulation and empirical data is improved by manipulating a model’s parameters points at two areas of analysis. First, it may suggest that the model is characterized by non-linear causal relationships among variables so that slightly different model’s calibrations yield very different emerging behaviors. Second, the causal structure at work may include positive feedback among variables and initial calibration of variables has a mounting weight in molding unfolding patterns of behavior. For example, as reported in Carrol and Harrison simulation study of organizational ecology’s density model of legitimization and competition (1994), the presence of positive feedback among variables generates behaviors that unfold in a way that is history-dependent. Given an environment in which populations compete that are characterized by different organizational forms, depending on researcher’s calibration of the time of entry of a new population in a simulated environment, not necessarily this new population will survive independently of its fitness to the environment. The second avenue to explore discrepancy between predictions and observed behaviors is the analysis of a model’s structure. That is, the causal relationships among variables that are deemed necessary to produce behaviors of interest. Different formalizations may exist for specific relationships and including in the model one or the other may have different behavioral implications. To revise formalization, researchers need to go back and compare the formal structure of the computer model and the real processes at work in the case study. This further investigation plays as a catalyst to define possible amendments to the theory. More interestingly, the analysis of the discrepancy unveils to researchers the perils and hazards that arise when formalizing descriptions that were originally provided in a verbal form. Formalization requires dubious and arguable interpretations but the very cycle that embeds collection of data, building verbal explanations, formalization of these explanations, simulation of formalizations and analysis of discrepancies between simulated and expected patterns of behaviors produces knowledge by forcing researchers to appreciate the consequences of different conceptualizations. In this case, knowledge building may be not directly, and immediately, on the empirical issue at hand but on the way of thinking at it. When playing with computer simulations generates cognitive dissonances between expected and observed patterns, the depth of our interpretations is potentially augmented by the opportunity to explore consequences of a rich and colorful repertoire of possible conceptualizations. The fairly intuitive idea here is that those formalizations that are directly obtained from descriptions sufficiently clear and less questionable are not good candidates to generate insightful dissonances; on the other hand, those formalizations that required a researcher’s translation of verbal descriptions into quantitative formulations are more debatable, more prone to conceal misinterpretation and hence good candidates for the analysis. Along this path, computer simulation experiments may reveal that and history-divergent simulation run has been observed because interviewed informants describe processes on the basis of existing formalized procedures whereas everyday activity is grounded on institutionalized informal and tacit routines which are different from those crystallized in official manuals, codes of rules or blueprints. THEORISING WITH COMPUTER SIMULATION: ABDUCTION AND FALSIFIABILITY What is theorizing about dynamics behavior, then, and how computer simulation may support the endeavor? We posit that generating theories of behavior requires to explore the plausibility of an explanation by defining sufficient conditions for a pattern to emerge. Computer simulation, by allowing researchers to play with calibration of parameters or to manipulate portions of a formal model, provides a virtual theoretical laboratory in which ‘what if’ analysis can be conducted to activate plausible but dormant alternative histories. This exercise is illuminates deep causal structures. In this light, theorizing is selecting a candidate causal structure that plausibly underpins an observed pattern of behavior, and to build a repertoire of alternative plausible histories that are connected to the same common deep common structure of causation (this can be obtained by manipulating a model’s parameters and structure). Thus, a formalized simulation model complements findings of field studies by facilitating a process of abduction. Abduction is an inference that goes from the observation of a fact to the hypothesis of a principle that explains the observed fact (Burks, 1964; Fann, 1970). In this vein, the model is a candidate explanation; had the world portrayed into the computer model to be true, observed patterns of behaviors would be reasonable. Differently, from typical research in social science conducted through statistical generalizations, in which falsification is focused on behavior, here, causal structure as well is object of direct falsification. Each field site is to be considered as an experiment in which a model’s findings can be confirmed, falsified or amended and the generalization sought after is analytical rather than statistical (Yin, 1994). In this perspective, having a detailed (often formalized) description of a causal structure and a description of a repertoire of plausible histories, a field researcher will have a variety of points in which the theoretical hypothesis, which crystallized in to the model, can be falsified (Bell and Bell,1980). POSITIVE FEEDBACK AND STRUCTURAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR: THE SYSTEM DYNAMICS APPROACH System dynamics (SD), which is connected to the work of Forrester (1961), grounds modeling on difference equations and impinges upon the assumption that the behavior of individuals that are embedded within a social system can be explained by the feedback nature of causal relationships that characterizes the structure of the system. Thus, SD approach aims at reducing emerging aggregate, and often puzzling, behaviors into underlying feedback causal structures. We suggest that SD has two features that makes this approach suitable for social sciences. First, SD illuminates the Inertia of social processes. The approach impinges upon a conceptual framework, and a symbolic language, that emphasizes the distinction between flow and stock variables. This distinction facilitates the representation of social processes that unfold over time. The role of stocks is one of accumulating results of past processes, these latter crystallised in flow variables. Stock variables represent the inertial features and properties of a social system that cannot be changed instantaneously (for example, the stock of accumulated knowledge or institutionalized routines). Stock variables embody the state of a social system in a particular time, independently of processes that are at work to change such a state. On the other hand, flow variables represent the processes that work to change the state of a social system (for example, the flows of knowledge distribution, which proceed from one area of a social system to another one, the processes of knowledge creation or the processes of knowledge erosion). Second, SD takes a feedback perspective and treats a social system as a complex system consisting of one or more feedback loops. The dynamic interplay of these feedback loops explains emerging non-linear behavior carried by multi-level actors in complex social systems, which is not necessarily intuitively understood, nor can be replicable using other conventional research methodologies. Behind the SD approach is an emphasis on the analysis of the relationship between a causal structure and a connected emerging behavior. This attitude to the exploration of system structure is appropriate to detect typical situations in which different observed patterns of behaviors may be generated by the same causal structure. For example, in presence of systems with strong stable attractors, the existence of a common causal structure explains equifinality, when different behaviors converge to the same steady state equilibrium from different initial conditions. On the other hand, a robust explanation in term of causal structure is particularly important in presence of positive feedbacks and systems with self-reinforcing properties. In this case, path-dependence and casual disturbances in the early history of a system may give raise to very different, often diverging, behaviors. Yet, the underlying causal structure is unchanged. When systems are characterized by pathdependence, actions and decisions produce different consequences depending on the time in history in which decisions are conceived of and actions are implemented. Yet, had researchers a clear framework that connects a given causal structure to a repertoire of alternative possible behaviors, observation of diverging behaviors in similar contexts may result less puzzling. Thus, detecting the presence and investigating the behavioral consequences of positive feedback loops facilitates theorizing in presence of historical inefficiency. The concept of historical inefficiency has been mentioned by Carrol and Harrison (1994) to indicate a social process “...with positive feedback (or self-reinforcement) that can generate outcomes that arise from "chance" rather than a systematic force.” Carrol and Harrison use the term ‘historical inefficiency’ as the contrary of ‘historical efficiency’ defined by March and Olsen (1989: 5-6) as follows: “Institutions and behavior are thought to evolve through some form of efficient historical process. An efficient historical process, in these terms, is one that moves rapidly to a unique solution, conditional on current environmental conditions, and is independent of the historical path.” When behavior is historical inefficient, actions and decisions become irreversible: actions or forces applied upon a social system are progressively ineffective as the pressure of existing positive feedback unfold. Reproducing in vitro these phenomena enriches theorizing with hypotheses concerning the points in time that were crucial in deciding the pattern of behavior of a specific. HOW COMPUTER SIMULATION INTERACT WITH FIELD STUDY To illustrate the way in which modeling and computer simulation support field studies in theorizing, we describe, as an example, a study of interaction dynamics between two clusters of firms geographically co-located that has been previously conducted by the author. We borrow this example of studies in organization theory and management, but the employed logic, we hope, may as well prove useful to other social scientists. Geographical clusters can be defined as spatially concentrated groups of small entrepreneurial firms competing in the same or related industries that are linked through vertical (buyer-supplier) or horizontal (alliance, resource sharing, etc.) relationships. What characterizes geographical clusters is that a complex network of firms is bound together in a social division of labor (Scott, 1982). Within geographical clusters, the development of a dense web of social relationships among firms facilitates the exchange of knowledge, in particular, of the most valuable knowledge that is highly tacit, difficult to replicate and not easily purchased (Keeble & Wilkinson 1999). Mollona and Presutti (2006) highlighted a process of passive internationalization that influence the internal equilibrium of industrial districts. Passive internationalization occurs when a high number of newly established small firms embedded in a dense web of social relationships establish in an area geographically close to a pre-existing network of firms that are themselves embedded in a web of social relationships. In this respect, Mollona and Presutti (2006) documented how in Italy, in the textile district of Val Vibrata, in the region of Abruzzo (centre of Italy), a newly emerged cluster of firms, which are owned by entrepreneurs that moved in Italy from China, competes with firms co-located into a pre-existing cluster. The two clusters have sharply distinct traits and, each is characterized by strong internal homogeneity that follows form shared language, similar modes of production and kinship relationships. Both the network of firms, which we call ‘Chinese’, and the preexisting network of firms, which we call ‘Italian’ are articulated in two populations: a population of suppliers and one of producers. Graph in figure 2 display the historical patterns of behavior of firms in the Val Vibrata district.
منابع مشابه
Archetypes for Organisational Safety
We propose a framework using system dynamics to model the dynamic behaviour of organisations in accident analysis. Most current accident analysis techniques are event-based and do not adequately capture the dynamic complexity and non-linear interactions that characterize accidents in complex systems. In this paper we propose a set of system safety archetypes that model common safety culture fla...
متن کاملDynamic Thinking Approach of Non-medical Use of Pharmaceutical Opioids
The unsteady increase in using pharmaceutical opioids is a phenomenon that has existed in human societies for a long time. Furthermore, the ever-growing trend of addiction to opioids affects communities in various economic, social, and cultural aspects. Since abusing pharmaceutical opioids is a complex dynamic problem, it is extremely difficult to recognize the factors influencing this abuse. T...
متن کاملDynamic Thinking Approach of Non-medical Use of Pharmaceutical Opioids
The unsteady increase in using pharmaceutical opioids is a phenomenon that has existed in human societies for a long time. Furthermore, the ever-growing trend of addiction to opioids affects communities in various economic, social, and cultural aspects. Since abusing pharmaceutical opioids is a complex dynamic problem, it is extremely difficult to recognize the factors influencing this abuse. T...
متن کاملطراحی مدل عاملمحور و کاربرد آن در باستانشناسی
The aim of this paper is to consider what constitutes agent-based modelling (ABM) and how this can relate to archaeological reasoning. The development and construction of ABM models is an essential prerequisite for most archaeological reasoning. Both directly and indirectly, archaeologists are making extensive use of ideas and methods in applications that derive from archaeological, anthropolog...
متن کاملExponential Random Graph Modeling for Micro-blog Network Analysis
Social network analysis is used to study complex networks by analyzing static structure and dynamic changes. Nowadays micro-blog as a new social media is becoming the most popular communication platform. How to capture micro-blog network structure especially dynamic structure poses more scientific interest. In this paper, we choose Chinese micro-blog, Sina weibo, on topic of diabetes as our tes...
متن کاملChallenges in Complex Systems Science
FuturICT foundations are social science, complex systems science, and ICT. The main concerns and challenges in the science of complex systems in the context of FuturICT are laid out in this paper with special emphasis on the Complex Systems route to Social Sciences. This include complex systems having: many heterogeneous interacting parts; multiple scales; complicated transition laws; unexpecte...
متن کامل